Abstract
The interactionist approach to second language acquisition has yielded a plethora of studies confirming the positive impact of interaction and corrective feedback on second language (L2) development. Nevertheless, only a few studies have attempted to investigate the development of L2 prosody using the interactionist approach. The current study contributes to this line of research by investigating the relationship between recasts and the production of primary stress in L2 English. Following a pretest-posttest design, 68 L1 Arabic speakers were randomly assigned to control and intervention groups. The pre- and posttest comprised sentence-completion and information-exchange tasks, whereas the intervention was a role-play task that dyads carried out with the researcher. The intervention group received a recast upon producing target words with misplaced primary stress, whereas the control group did not receive any corrective feedback. The results of acoustic analyses, which focused on syllable duration, intensity, and pitch, indicated a positive relationship between recasts and development of primary stress placement. The results were also supported by expert listener judgments. The findings suggest that interaction and implicit corrective feedback play a positive role in the development of lexical stress.
References
Almbark, R., Bouchhioua, N., & Hellmuth, S. (2014). Acquiring the phonetics and phonology of English word stress: Comparing learners from different L1 backgrounds. Concordia Working Papers in Applied Linguistics, 5, 19-35.
Alrajeh, N. (2011). Digital processing of stress in standard Arabic. Journal of King Saud University-Languages and Translation, 23(2), 65-68. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jksult.2011.03.001
Archibald, J. (1997). The acquisition of English stress by speakers of nonaccentual languages: Lexical storage versus computation. Linguistics, 35(1), 167-181. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/ling.1997.35.1.167
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects struc-ture for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 255-278. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1-48. DOI: https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01
Beckman, M. (1986). Stress and non-stress accent. Foris. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110874020
Best, C. T., & Tyler, M. D. (2007). Nonnative and second-language speech perception. In O. S. Bohn & M. J. Munro (Eds.), Language experience in second language speech learning: In honor of James Emil Flege (pp. 13-24). John Benjamins. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1075/lllt.17.07bes
Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2018). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 6.0.28) [Computer program]. http://www.praat.org/
Braidi, S. M. (2002). Reexamining the role of recasts in native‐speaker/nonnative‐speaker interactions. Language Learning, 52(1), 1-42. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9922.00176
Brown, D. (2016). The type and linguistic foci of oral corrective feedback in the L2 classroom: A meta-analysis. Language Teaching Research, 20(4), 436-458. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168814563200
Bryfonski, L., & Ma, X. (2020). Effects of implicit versus explicit corrective feedback on Mandarin tone acquisition in a SCMC learning environment. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 42, 61-88. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263119000317
Carpenter, H., Jeon, K. S., MacGregor, D., & Mackey, A. (2006). Learners’ interpretations of recasts. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 209-236. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263106060104
Colantoni, L., Marasco, O., Steele, J., & Sunara, S. (2014). Learning to realize prosodic prominence in L2 French and Spanish. In R. T Miller et al. (Eds.), Selected proceedings of the 2012 second language research forum: Building bridges between disciplines (pp. 15-29). Cascadilla Proceedings Project.
Cunnings, I., & Finlayson, I. (2015). Mixed effects modeling and longitudinal data analysis. In L. Plonsky (Ed.), Advancing quantitative methods in second language research (pp. 159-181). Routledge. DOI: https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315870908-8
Cutler, A. (2005). Lexical stress. In D. B. Pisoni & R. E. Remez (Eds.), The handbook of speech perception (pp. 264-289). Blackwell. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470757024.ch11
de Jong, K., & Zawaydeh, B. A. (2002). Comparing stress, lexical focus, and segmental focus: Patterns of variation in Arabic vowel duration. Journal of Phonetics, 30, 53-75. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1006/jpho.2001.0151
Derwing, T. M., & Munro, M. J. (2015). Pronunciation fundamentals: Evidence-based perspectives for L2 teaching and research. John Benjamins. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1075/lllt.42
Deterding, D. (2001). The measurement of rhythm: A comparison of Singa-pore and British English. Journal of Phonetics, 29, 217-230. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1006/jpho.2001.0138
Educational Testing Service. (2010). Linking TOEFL iBT scores to IELTS scores: A research report. Available at https://www.ets.org/s/toefl/pdf/linking_toefl_ibt_scores_to_ielts_scores.pdf
Ellis, R. (2016). Focus on form: A critical review. Language Teaching Re-search, 20(3), 405-428. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168816628627
Field, J. (2005). Intelligibility and the listener: The role of lexical stress. TESOL Quarterly, 39(3), 399-423. DOI: https://doi.org/10.2307/3588487
Flege, J. E. (1995). Two procedures for training a novel second language phonetic contrast. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16(4), 425-442. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716400066029
Flege, J. E., & Port, R. (1981). Cross-language phonetic interference: Arabic to English. Language and Speech, 24(2), 125-146. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/002383098102400202
Gass, S. M. (1988). Integrating research areas: A framework for second language studies. Applied Linguistics, 9(2), 198-217. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/9.2.198
Gass, S. M., & Mackey, A. (2006). Input, interaction and output: An overview. AILA Review, 19(1), 3-17. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1075/aila.19.03gas
Gass, S. M., & Mackey, A. (2015). Input, interaction, and output in second language acquisition. In B. VanPatten & J. Williams (Eds.), Theories in second language acquisition: An introduction (pp. 180-206). Routledge.
Goo, J. (2012). Corrective feedback and working memory capacity in interaction-driven L2 learning. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 34, 445-474. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263112000149
Goo, J., & Mackey, A. (2013). The case against the case against recasts. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35(1), 127-165. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263112000708
Gordon, M., & Roettger, T. (2017). Acoustic correlates of word stress: A cross-linguistic survey. Linguistics Vanguard, 3(1), 1-21. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/lingvan-2017-0007
Hellmuth, S. (2013). Phonology. In J. Owens (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of Arabic linguistics (pp. 45-70). Oxford University Press. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199764136.013.0003
Isaacs, T., & Trofimovich, P. (2012). Deconstructing comprehensibility. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 34(3), 475-505. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263112000150
Kang, O., Rubin, D. O. N., & Pickering, L. (2010). Suprasegmental measures of accentedness and judgments of language learner proficiency in oral English. Modern Language Journal, 94(4), 554-566. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4781.2010.01091.x
Kartchava, E. & Ammar, A. (2014). The noticeability and effectiveness of corrective feedback in relation to target type. Language Teaching Research, 18, 428-452. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/1362168813519373
Kochanski, G., Grabe, E., Coleman, J., & Rosner, B. (2005). Loudness predicts prominence: Fundamental frequency lends little. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 118(2), 1038-1054. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1923349
Ladefoged, P. (2006). A course in phonetics. Thomson Wadsworth.
Lee, A. H., & Lyster, R. (2016). The effects of corrective feedback on instructed L2 speech perception. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38(1), 35-64. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263115000194
Leeman, J. (2003). Recasts and second language development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25(1), 37-63. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263103000020
Lefcheck, J. S. (2016). piecewiseSEM: Piecewise structural equation modelling in r for ecology, evolution, and systematics. Methods in Ecology and Evolution, 7(5), 573-579. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.12512
Lenth, R. V. (2022). emmeans: Estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means. Retrieved from https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=emmeans
Leung, A. H. C., Young-Scholten, M., Almurashi, W., Ghadanfari, S., Nash, C., & Outhwaite, O. (2021). (Mis) perception of consonant clusters and short vowels in English as a foreign language. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1515/iral-2021-0030
Lewis, C., & Deterding, D. (2018). Word stress and pronunciation teaching in English as a lingua franca contexts. CATESOL Journal, 30(1), 161-176. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-58542-0_41-1
Li, S. (2010). The effectiveness of corrective feedback in SLA: A meta-analysis. Language Learning, 60(2), 309-365. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2010.00561.x
Long, M. H. (1990). The least a second language acquisition theory needs to explain. TESOL Quarterly, 24(4), 649-666. DOI: https://doi.org/10.2307/3587113
Long, M. H. (1996). The role of the linguistic environment in second language acquisition. In W. C. Ritchie & T. K. Bhatia (Eds.), Handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 413-468). Academic Press. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012589042-7/50015-3
Long, M. H. (2007). Problems in SLA. Lawrence Erlbaum.
Mackey, A., Abbuhl, R., & Gass, S. M. (2012). Interactionist approach. In S. M. Gass & A. Mackey (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 7-23). Routledge. DOI: https://doi.org/10.4324/9781410612564
Mackey, A., Gass, S. M., & McDonough, K. (2000). How do learners perceive interactional feedback? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 22(4), 471-497. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263100004010
Mackey, A., & Goo, J. (2007). Interaction research in SLA: A meta-analysis and research synthesis. In A. Mackey (Ed.), Conversational interaction in second language acquisition: A collection of empirical studies (pp. 407-452). Oxford University Press.
Munro, M. J. (1993). Productions of English vowels by native speakers of Arabic: Acoustic measurements and accentedness ratings. Language and Speech, 36(1), 39-66. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1177/002383099303600103
Mustafawi, E. (2018). Arabic phonology. In E. Benmamoun & R. Bassiouney (Eds.), The Routledge handbook of Arabic linguistics (pp. 11-31). Routledge. DOI: https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315147062-2
Nguyễn, T. A. T., Ingram, C. L. J., & Pensalfini, J. R. (2008). Prosodic transfer in Vietnamese acquisition of English contrastive stress patterns. Journal of Phonetics, 36(1), 158-190. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2007.09.001
Parlak, Ö., & Ziegler, N. (2017). The impact of recasts on the development of primary stress in a synchronous computer-mediated environment. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 39(2), 257-285. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263116000310
Peirce, J. W., Gray, J. R., Simpson, S., MacAskill, M. R., Höchenberger, R., Sogo, H., Kastman, & E., Lindeløv, J. (2019). PsychoPy2: Experiments in behavior made easy. Behavior Research Methods. DOI: https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-018-01193-y
Philp, J. (2003). Constraints on “noticing the gap”: Nonnative speakers’ no-ticing of recasts in NS-NNS interaction. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 25, 99-126. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263103000044
Quené, H. (2015). Package ‘hqmisc’. https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/hqmisc/hqmisc.pdf
R Core Team. (2022). R: A language and environment for statistical computing (Version 4.2.0). R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/
Saito, K. (2013). Reexamining effects of form-focused instruction on L2 pronunciation development: The role of explicit phonetic information. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 35(1), 1-29. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263112000666
Saito, K. (2015). Communicative focus on second language phonetic form: Teaching Japanese learners to perceive and produce English /ɹ/ with-out explicit instruction. Applied Psycholinguistics, 36(2), 377-409. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716413000271
Saito, K., & Lyster, R. (2012). Effects of form-focused instruction and corrective feedback on L2 pronunciation development of /ɹ/ by Japanese learners of English. Language Learning, 62, 595-633. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9922.2011.00639.x
Saito, K., & Plonsky, L. (2019). Effects of second language pronunciation teaching revisited: A proposed measurement framework and meta‐analysis. Language Learning, 69(3), 652-708. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/lang.12345
Saito, K., Trofimovich, P., & Isaacs, T. (2016). Second language speech production: Investigating linguistic correlates of comprehensibility and accentedness for learners at different ability levels. Applied Psycholinguistics, 37(2), 217-240. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716414000502
Saito, K., & Wu, X. (2014). Communicative focus on form and second language suprasegmental learning: Teaching Cantonese learners to per-ceive Mandarin tones. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 36(4), 647-680. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S0272263114000114
Scheuer, S., & Horgues, C. (2019). Corrective feedback in English/French spoken tandem interactions. In C. Tardieu & C. Horgues (Eds.), Redefining tandem language and culture learning in higher education (pp. 147-160). Routledge. DOI: https://doi.org/10.4324/9780429505898-10
Schmidt, R. W. (2001). Attention. In P. Robinson (Ed.), Cognition and second language instruction (pp. 1-32). Cambridge University Press. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139524780.003
Sheen, Y. (2004). Corrective feedback and learner uptake in communicative classrooms across instructional settings. Language Teaching Research, 8(3), 263-300. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1191/1362168804lr146oa
Sheen, Y. (2006). Exploring the relationship between characteristics of recasts and learner uptake. Language Teaching Research, 10(4), 361-392. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1191/1362168806lr203oa
Swain, M. (2005). The output hypothesis: Theory and research. In E. Hinkel (Ed.), Handbook of research in second language teaching and learning (pp. 471-483). Lawrence Erlbaum.
Watson, J. C. (2007). The phonology and morphology of Arabic. Oxford Uni-versity Press.
West, B. T., Welch, K. B., & Galecki, A. T. (2015). Linear mixed models: A practical guide using statistical software. CRC Press. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1201/b17198
Winter, B. (2013). Linear models and linear mixed effects models in R with linguistic applications. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.48550/arXiv.1308.5499
Zhang, Y., Nissen, S. L., & Francis, A. L. (2008). Acoustic characteristics of English lexical stress produced by native Mandarin speakers. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 123(6), 4498-4513. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2902165
Ziegler, N. (2016). Synchronous computer-mediated communication and interaction: A meta-analysis. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 38(3), 553-586. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/S027226311500025X
Zuraiq, W., & Sereno, J. (2021). Production of English lexical stress by Arabic speakers. In R. Wayland (Ed.), Second language speech learning: Theoretical and empirical progress (pp. 290-311). Cambridge University Press. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108886901.012
License
Copyright (c) 2024 Özgür Parlak
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
1.1 The Author hereby warrants that he/she is the owner of all the copyright and other intellectual property rights in the Work and that, within the scope of the present Agreement, the paper does not infringe the legal rights of another person. The owner of the copyright work also warrants that he/she is the sole and original creator thereof and that is not bound by any legal constraints in regard to the use or sale of the work.
1.2. The Publisher warrants that is the owner of the PRESSto platform for open access journals, hereinafter referred to as the PRESSto Platform.
2. The Author grants the Publisher non-exclusive and free of charge license to unlimited use worldwide over an unspecified period of time in the following areas of exploitation:
2.1. production of multiple copies of the Work produced according to the specific application of a given technology, including printing, reproduction of graphics through mechanical or electrical means (reprography) and digital technology;
2.2. marketing authorisation, loan or lease of the original or copies thereof;
2.3. public performance, public performance in the broadcast, video screening, media enhancements as well as broadcasting and rebroadcasting, made available to the public in such a way that members of the public may access the Work from a place and at a time individually chosen by them;
2.4. inclusion of the Work into a collective work (i.e. with a number of contributions);
2.5. inclusion of the Work in the electronic version to be offered on an electronic platform, or any other conceivable introduction of the Work in its electronic version to the Internet;
2.6. dissemination of electronic versions of the Work in its electronic version online, in a collective work or independently;
2.7. making the Work in the electronic version available to the public in such a way that members of the public may access the Work from a place and at a time individually chosen by them, in particular by making it accessible via the Internet, Intranet, Extranet;
2.8. making the Work available according to appropriate license pattern Attribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0) as well as another language version of this license or any later version published by Creative Commons.
3. The Author grants the Publisher permission to reproduce a single copy (print or download) and royalty-free use and disposal of rights to compilations of the Work and these compilations.
4. The Author grants the Publisher permission to send metadata files related to the Work, including to commercial and non-commercial journal-indexing databases.
5. The Author represents that, on the basis of the license granted in the present Agreement, the Publisher is entitled and obliged to:
5.1. allow third parties to obtain further licenses (sublicenses) to the Work and to other materials, including derivatives thereof or compilations made, based on or including the Work, whereas the provisions of such sub-licenses will be the same as with the Attribution 4.0 International (CC BY 4.0) Creative Commons sub-license or another language version of this license, or any later version of this license published by Creative Commons;
5.2. make the Work available to the public in such a way that members of the public may access the Work from a place and at a time individually chosen by them, without any technological constraints;
5.3. appropriately inform members of the public to whom the Work is to be made available about sublicenses in such a way as to ensure that all parties are properly informed (appropriate informing messages).
6. Because of the royalty-free provision of services of the Author (resulting from the scope of obligations stipulated in the present Agreement), the Author shall not be entitled to any author’s fee due and payable on the part of the Publisher (no fee or royalty is payable by the Publisher to the Author).
7.1. In the case of third party claims or actions for indemnity against the Publisher owing to any infractions related to any form of infringement of intellectual property rights protection, including copyright infringements, the Author is obliged to take all possible measures necessary to protect against these claims and, when as a result of legal action, the Publisher, or any third party licensed by the Publisher to use the Work, will have to abandon using the Work in its entirety or in part or, following a court ruling in a legal challenge, to pay damages to a third party, whatever the legal basis
7.2. The Author will immediately inform the Publisher about any damage claims related to intellectual property infringements, including the author’s proprietary rights pertaining to a copyrighted work, filed against the Author. of liability, the Author is obliged to redress the damage resulting from claims made by third party, including costs and expenditures incurred in the process.
7.3. To all matters not settled herein provisions of the Polish Civil Code and the Polish Copyright and Related Rights Act shall apply.