Circadian rhythms and second language performance
Cover
PDF

Keywords

circadian rhythm
chronotype
aptitude
vocabulary

How to Cite

de Bot, K., & Fang, F. (2017). Circadian rhythms and second language performance. Studies in Second Language Learning and Teaching, 7(1), 47–60. https://doi.org/10.14746/ssllt.2017.7.1.3

Number of views: 461


Number of downloads: 365

Abstract

Human behavior is not constant over the hours of the day, and there are considerable individual differences. Some people raise early and go to bed early and have their peek performance early in the day (“larks”) while others tend to go to bed late and get up late and have their best performance later in the day (“owls”). In this contribution we report on three projects on the role of chronotype (CT) in language processing and learning. The first study (de Bot, 2013) reports on the impact of CT on language learning aptitude and word learning. The second project was reported in Fang (2015) and looks at CT and executive functions, in particular inhibition as measured by variants of the Stroop test. The third project aimed at assessing lexical access in L1 and L2 at preferred and non-preferred times of the day. The data suggest that there are effects of CT on language learning and processing. There is a small effect of CT on language aptitude and a stronger effect of CT on lexical access in the first and second language. The lack of significance for other tasks is mainly caused by the large interindividual and intraindividual variation.
https://doi.org/10.14746/ssllt.2017.7.1.3
PDF

References

Blatter, K., & Cajochen, C. (2007). Circadian rhythms in cognitive performance: Methodological constraints, protocols, theoretical underpinnings. Physiology & Behavior, 90, 196-208.

de Bot, K. (2013). Circadian rhythms and second language development. International Journal of Bilingualism, 19(2), 142-155. doi: 10.1177/1367006913489201.

Dörnyei, Z. (2009). The psychology of second language acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Dörnyei, Z., & Skehan, P. (2003). Individual differences in second language learning. In C. J. Doughty & M. H. Long (Eds.), The handbook of second language acquisition (pp. 589-630). Oxford: Blackwell.

Díaz-Morales, J. F. (2007). Morning and evening-types: Exploring their personality styles. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(4), 769-778.

Dijk, D. J., & von Schantz, M. (2005). Timing and consolidation of human sleep, wakefulness, and performance by a symphony of oscillators. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 20(4), 279-290.

Ebbinghaus, H. (1964). Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology (H. A. Rugen & C. E. Business, Trans.). New York: Dover. (Original work published 1885)

Ellis, R. (1994). The study of second language acquisition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Ellis, R. (2004). Individual differences in second language learning. In A. Davies & C. Elder (Eds.), The handbook of applied linguistics (pp. 525-551). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

Fang, F. (2015). Circadian Rhythm and second language acquisition (Unpublished MA thesis). University of Groningen, the Netherlands.

Farrell, A., Sood, A., Dewsnap, B., & Schmitt, J. (2013). Student morningness-eveningness type and performance: Does class timing matter? In S. Sezgin, E. Karaosmanoglu, & A. B. Elmadag Bas (Eds.), EMAC 2013 : 42nd annual EMAC conference: Lost in translation.

Giampietro, M., & Cavallera, G. M. (2007). Morning and evening types and creative thinking. Personality and Individual Differences, 42(3), 453-463.

Hahn, C., Cowell, J. M., Wiprzycka, U. J., Goldstein, D., Ralph, M., Hasher, L., & Zelazo, P. D. (2012). Circadian rhythms in executive function during the transition to adolescence: The effect of synchrony between chronotype and time of day. Developmental Science, 15(3), 408-416.

Hogan, M. J., Kelly, C. A., Verrier, D., Newell, J., Hasher, L., & Robertson, I. H. (2009). Optimal time-of-day and consolidation of learning in younger and older adults. Experimental Aging Research, 35(1), 107-128.

Kreitzman, L., & Foster, R. (2011). The rhythms of life: The biological clocks that control the daily lives of every living thing. London: Profile Books.

May, C. P., & Hasher, L. (1998). Synchrony effects in inhibitory control over thought and action. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24(2), 363-379.

Monsch, A. U., Bondi, M. W., Butters, N., Salmon, D. P., Katzman, R., & Thal, L. J. (1992). Comparisons of verbal fluency tasks in the detection of dementia of the Alzheimer type. Archives of Neurology, 49(12), 1253-1258.

Portocarrero, J. S., Burright, R. G., & Donovick, P. J. (2007).Vocabulary and verbal fluency of bilingual and monolingual college students. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22(3), 415-422.

Roenneberg, T., Wirz-Justice, A., & Merrow, M. (2003). Life between clocks: Daily temporal patterns of human chronotypes. Journal of Biological Rhythms, 18(1), 80-90.

Rosselli, M., Ardila, A., & Santisi, M. (2002). Stroop effects in Spanish/English bilinguals. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8(6), 819-827.

Schmidt, C., Collette, F., Cajochen, C., & Peigneux, P. (2007). A time to think: Circadian rhythms in human cognition. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24(7), 755-789.